nervous system

11 The nervous system


Medical students often approach the neurological examination with trepidation, no doubt partly because of the complexities of the nervous system and partly because of the difficulties sometimes experienced in attempting anatomical localisation on the basis of abnormal physical signs. The problem for the student, however, often begins with a failure to acquire the skills necessary to elicit those signs. If they are not identified correctly, mistakes in interpretation and diagnosis inevitably follow.


This chapter summarises the examination of the central and peripheral nervous systems, although it will seldom be necessary to examine all the areas covered. Selection is influenced partly by the patient’s history but also by cooperation, conscious state and level of fatigue. Certain examination techniques demand a good deal of both patient and examiner and if responses become erratic it is better to return to the examination later. Students, and sometimes doctors, are prone to examine only those areas immediately accessible with the patient supine. Remember to turn the patient over in order to assess the spine and the muscles of the shoulder and pelvic girdles. Always record your findings in full, avoiding irritating acronyms (e.g. PERLA for pupils equal, reacting to light and accommodation) and, if your examination has been limited, state exactly what you have done (rather than just ‘CNS’ followed by a tick). Remember that physical signs can alter, sometimes rapidly, and repeating your examination can give you useful insight into the mechanisms of certain disorders.



The cortex



STRUCTURE AND FUNCTION


On the basis of differences in histological structure, distinct areas can be identified within the cerebral cortex (Fig. 11.1). Tracts within the cortex comprise efferent pathways such as the pyramidal system, afferent pathways such as the thalamocortical projections, association fibres passing from regions within the hemisphere and commissural fibres connecting regions contralateral to one another. Surrounding the primary cortical areas for movement, sensation and vision are the cortical association areas. For example, the lateral geniculate body projects not just to the visual cortex (area 17) but also to areas 18 and 19, parts of the visual association cortex (Fig. 11.1).



The frontal lobe is separated from the parietal lobe posteriorly by the central (rolandic) sulcus, while the temporal lobe lies below the lateral (sylvian) sulcus. The boundaries of the parietal, temporal and occipital lobes are not defined by a specific sulcus (Fig. 11.2).



The brain is supplied by paired internal carotid and vertebral arteries. The former terminate in the anterior and middle cerebral arteries, the latter in the basilar artery, which ends by forming the posterior cerebral arteries (Fig. 11.3). An anastomotic system at the base of the brain (the circle of Willis) connects these various components. The lateral surface of the cortex is supplied predominantly by the middle cerebral artery. The anterior cerebral artery supplies a strip of cortex spanning its superior margin, while the posterior cerebral artery supplies the occipital lobe and the inferior aspect of the temporal lobe (Fig. 11.4). Normal cerebral blood flow, at approximately 55  ml/100  g/min, represents approximately 15% of cardiac output. The level of blood flow is largely dependent on the Pco2 of arterial blood. Vasodilatation and increased flow occur as Pco2 rises. During a specific task orientated to speech, vision, hearing or motor activity, a focal increase in flow occurs in the appropriate part of the cortex.




The ventricular system contains cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) which originates predominantly in the choroid plexuses of the lateral ventricles, then circulates through the third ventricle and aqueduct before reaching the fourth ventricle. The CSF exits through the foramina of the fourth ventricle and is eventually reabsorbed through the arachnoid villae. The rate of CSF production is approximately 120  ml/24  h.


Acquisition of memory requires a number of stages. All data, whether visual or verbal, are recorded temporarily in a short-term pool. A selective and active process then passes some of the data into a long-term memory store. Finally, an active process of retrieval restores the memory to consciousness. Conventionally, memory is divided into immediate, recent and remote components, although these divisions are not absolute. Immediate or short-term memory lasts a few seconds; recent memory relates to activities or events occurring within a few hours or days and remote memory to events of the past, for example, the individual’s youth. Structures particularly associated with learning storage include the hippocampi, the mamillary bodies and the dorsomedial nuclei of the thalami – the limbic system. Remote memory, however, can be retrieved even if these structures are damaged, suggesting that it is stored predominantly in the association cortex appropriate to the memory modality.


Visuospatial ability is dependent mainly on nondominant parietal lobe function. Language function is located in the left hemisphere in around 99% of right-handed individuals. For left-handed individuals, some 60% have language dominance in the left hemisphere, with 40% in the right hemisphere. Something like 80% of all left-handed individuals have mixed dominance, with language represented in both hemispheres. Within the hemisphere, a posteriorly placed area (Wernicke) is concerned with the comprehension of spoken language and an anterior area (Broca) with language output. The two are connected by the arcuate fasciculus (Fig. 11.5). The integration of the auditory and visual data required for reading and writing is achieved by the angular gyrus (area 39) (Fig. 11.1).





Although certain functions can be localised to specific cortical areas, other aspects of higher cortical function are represented more diffusely.



SYMPTOMS


Many of the symptoms arising from a disorder of higher cortical function will be more evident to a close friend or relative than to the patient. Areas to cover, although some will be more evident during formal examination, are the following.










MEMORY






INTELLIGENCE


Testing a patient’s knowledge and abstract thinking must be performed in the light of their social background. Inherent in all assessments of intelligence is an estimate of the patient’s premorbid ability.






Constructional ability


Constructional ability can be tested by asking the patient to copy designs of increasing complexity (Fig. 11.7). A scoring system can be devised, low values for which correlate well with the presence of brain damage. When assessing the patient’s drawing, look for evidence of unilateral neglect, suggesting the probability of a contralateral parietal lobe syndrome.





SPEECH AND SPEECH DEFECTS


Determine the patient’s handedness. Asking which hand is used for writing is insufficient because some left-handed individuals have been taught to write with their right hand. Ask which hand is used for holding a knife, using a hair brush, using a screwdriver or for playing racket sports. Check on the family history of handedness.












PRAXIS


Apraxia is a disorder of skilled movement not attributable to weakness, incoordination, sensory loss or a failure of comprehension. The problem in movement may be confined to the limbs, to the trunk or even to the buccofacial musculature. Historically, apraxia was separated into two main categories. In ideational apraxia, destruction of the motor programming area in the supramarginal gyrus impairs the conception of an action, leading to defecits in using tools or performing actions to verbal commands, though sparing the ability to mimic. Ideomotor apraxia results from separation of the motor programming area from the motor and premotor areas. Here conception of the action is spared but not the ability to perform it. The nomenclature of apraxia has become much more complex, along with the recognition that ideational apraxia is a less definable entity.


Start by asking the patient to carry out a particular task (e.g. ‘pretend to use a screwdriver’). If the patient is unable to perform the task, do it yourself and then ask the patient to copy your movement. If a response is still not forthcoming, provide the object in question and ask the patient to demonstrate its use. These three tiers of command are in descending order of difficulty for the apraxic patient. Instructions that will test relevant movements include ‘put out your tongue’, ‘pretend to whistle’, ‘salute’ and ‘show how you would use a toothbrush’. For whole body movements, ask the patient to stand to attention or stand as if about to start dancing. A more complex motor sequence is tested by asking the patient to go through a series of related movements. For example, taking the cap off a toothpaste tube, squeezing the toothpaste onto a brush, then replacing the cap.







Clinical application








APHASIA


Nonfluent speech is associated with anterior hemisphere lesions and fluent speech with posterior hemisphere lesions. Further differentiation is based on the results of testing comprehension, repetition and naming (Fig. 11.12).













APRAXIA


The pathway involved in performing a skilled task to command begins in the auditory association cortex of the dominant hemisphere then passes to the parietal association cortex, subsequently travelling forwards to the premotor cortex and finally the motor cortex itself. Interruption of this pathway at any point results in an ideomotor apraxia affecting both the dominant and nondominant hands (Fig. 11.13). The pathway from the dominant to the nondominant premotor cortex (D–D) passes through the anterior corpus callosum. A lesion there will produce an apraxia confined to the left hand. Whole body movements tend to be relatively spared even when limb ideomotor apraxia is substantial. Ideational apraxia is usually the consequence of bilateral hemisphere lesions or predominant left hemisphere dysfunction.







The psychiatric assessment


Make sure that the patient understands who you are, and the purpose of the interview. Privacy is particularly important when sensitive issues are being explored. To begin with, avoid making notes, as this can detract from the relationship you are trying to establish with the patient. During this preliminary phase, observation of the patient’s posture, gestures and facial expression may provide information regarding mood and feeling. The depressed patient appears apathetic, has little expressivity and may well be reluctant to discuss the history. The agitated patient is restless.



HISTORY OF PRESENT CONDITION


This proceeds in much the same way as history-taking from a patient with a physical complaint. Indeed, physical symptoms often predominate in those individuals with a primary psychiatric illness. Try to establish when the patient last felt well, as a means of determining the overall length of the history and as a means then of establishing the chronological order of subsequent symptoms. If necessary, interrupt the patient if there is digression into other areas, for example current social issues, through making clear that you are interested in those issues, and will wish to return to them later. Sometimes directive questions are needed to focus the patient’s attention on a particular symptom, for example headache, in order to explore that symptom in greater detail. As the history proceeds, open questions will be partly replaced by closed questions, answerable by a simple yes or no response. Sometimes signs of emotional distress may appear as certain issues are raised. Rather than ignoring these, gently probe them, even if this temporarily diverts the course of the history.


Quite often, patients only indirectly refer to stressful issues by giving oblique reference to them in the course of describing their physical symptoms. Try to pick up these cues and develop the relevant issue. Failure to detect them will deter the patient from discussing them further.


Many symptoms are common to both physical and psychiatric illness but others are more specifically within the territory of psychiatry.



SPECIFIC SYMPTOMS




Abnormal thoughts


These will be elicited only by sensitive questioning. The patient can be understandably reluctant to reveal certain abnormal thoughts. It may be apparent from the interview that the patient’s thought pattern is difficult to follow or that abnormal thoughts have pervaded the conversation. Ask patients about paranoid ideas, in other words, whether they feel people are against them. Ask whether certain thoughts or ideas regularly intrude into their thinking, or whether they believe their thoughts are being interfered with or influenced by external agencies. Thought disorders include the following.





Abnormal perceptions


These are auditory or visual phenomena that other individuals are not aware of.


Hallucinations are experiences that have no objective equivalent to explain them. They are predominantly visual or auditory but can occur in other forms, for example of smell or taste in patients with complex partial seizures. Visual hallucinations can be unformed, for example an ill-defined pattern of lights, or formed, the individual then describing people or animals, often of a frightening aspect. Visual hallucinations are more often a feature of an organic brain syndrome, e.g. delirium tremens or adverse drug reaction, than a functional psychosis, e.g. schizophrenia. Auditory hallucinations are also either unformed or formed. They are found more often in the functional psychoses than in organic brain disease. The voices can take on a persecutory quality in schizophrenia and an accusatory element in depression. In déja and jamais vu, intense feelings of a relived experience or a sensation of strangeness in familiar surroundings occur, respectively. Both can be a feature of everyday life but when pathological are usually epileptic. Illusions are misinterpretations of an external reality: all of us have this when watching a magician at work. In depersonalisation, the individual feels a detachment from the normal sense of self; in derealisation, the individual feels a detachment from the external world. Both occur in neurotic illnesses but also, periodically, in normal individuals.





THE PERSONAL HISTORY






















Headache and facial pain



HEADACHE


Headache is a common complaint. Often the history suffices to separate the more serious causes. The length of history is particularly important, rather then necessarily the severity of the pain. Critical issues are whether the pain is continuous or intermittent and, if the latter, what is the duration of individual attacks. There may be particular precipitants for attacks, e.g. alcohol triggering an attack of cluster headache. Accompanying symptoms should be sought, e.g. nausea or vomiting. As for any pain, it is important to determine whether there are relieving factors. Patients often find it difficult to describe the quality of their pain – it may be helpful to provide them with a list of alternatives.






The olfactory (first) nerve



STRUCTURE AND FUNCTION


The olfactory epithelium contains specialised receptor cells and free nerve endings, the latter derived from the first and second divisions of the trigeminal nerve. Unmyelinated axons from the receptor cells traverse the cribriform plate before synapsing in the olfactory bulb. From here, the olfactory tract passes backwards, dividing into lateral and medial roots in the region of the anterior perforated substance. The more important lateral root projects predominantly to the uncus of the ipsilateral temporal lobe.


Molecules derived from particular odours are absorbed into the mucus covering the olfactory epithelium. From here they diffuse via ciliary processes to the terminal processes of the receptor cells where they bind reversibly to receptor sites. This initiates an action potential in the olfactory nerve with a firing frequency related to the intensity of the stimulus.


Women have a more sensitive sense of smell than men. In both sexes, smell sensitivity declines with age. Many healthy individuals have difficulty naming or describing the quality of a particular odour even though they can distinguish it from others. The value of a particular odour for the testing of olfactory nerve function is determined principally by how selectively it stimulates the specialised receptor cells rather than the free trigeminal endings. Odours stimulating the latter include peppermint, camphor, ammonia, menthol and anisol. Highly selective stimulants of olfactory nerve endings include β-phenyl ethyl alcohol, methyl cyclopentenolone and isovaleric acid. Coffee, cinnamon and chocolate are useful everyday odours for the bedside testing of smell.







SYMPTOMS


The disturbances of smell that occur are defined in the ‘symptoms and signs’ box. For loss of smell, determine whether it is bilateral or unilateral.





The optic (second) nerve



STRUCTURE AND FUNCTION


Two types of retinal photoreceptor, rods and cones, have been identified in humans. At the fovea only cones are found, with rods predominating in the periphery. Fibres from the nasal aspect of the fovea pass directly to the optic disc. Fibres from above and below the fovea pass almost directly but fibres from the temporal border pass almost vertically, both superiorly and inferiorly, before arching around the other foveal fibres on their way to the optic disc (Fig. 11.14). Axons in the papillomacular bundle originate in the cones of the fovea and occupy a substantial proportion of the temporal aspect of the optic disc. Fibres from the superior and inferior parts of the periphery of the retina occupy corresponding areas in the optic nerve. As the papillomacular bundle approaches the chiasm, it moves centrally. The crossing, nasal, macular fibres occupy the central and posterior part of the chiasm.



The superior peripheral nasal fibres cross more posteriorly than the ventral fibres, which loop slightly into the terminal part of the opposite optic nerve (Fig. 11.15). Crossed and uncrossed fibres are arranged in alternate layers in the lateral geniculate body. The optic radiation extends from the lateral geniculate body to the visual (striate) cortex, area 17 (Fig. 11.1). The ventral fibres of the radiation loop forward towards the tip of the temporal lobe. The visual cortex is situated along the superior and inferior margins of the calcarine fissure, extending approximately 1.5  cm around the posterior pole. The macular representation lies posteriorly, with dorsal and ventral retina above and below the fissure, respectively. The unpaired outer 30° of the temporal field is represented in the contralateral hemisphere at the anterior limit of the striate cortex (Fig. 11.16).




Conditions of high (photopic) illumination activate cone photoreceptors, providing high spatial resolution and colour vision sense. Colour appreciation depends on three types of cone with spectral sensitivities spanning the range of colour vision. Low-illumination (scotopic) responses are mediated by rods.





VISUAL ACUITY


Visual acuity is tested in conditions of high illumination, producing a measure of cone function. The Snellen chart is used for testing distance vision. With the patient at the distance shown above a particular line, the visual angle subtended by a letter in that line is 5′ and by individual components of the letter, 1′ (Fig. 11.17).



Seat or stand the patient 6  m from the card. Ask the patient to cover each eye in turn and find which is the smallest line of print that can be read comfortably. The visual acuity is then expressed as the ratio of the distance between the patient and the card (usually 6  m), to the figure on the chart immediately above the smallest visible line. An acuity of 6/18, therefore, indicates that, at 6  m from the chart, the patient is able to read down only to the 18  m line. Make sure the patient wears glasses if they contain a distance correction. If the patient’s glasses are not available, reading through a pinhole will partly correct for any myopia. If unable to read the 60  m line at 6  m, the patient can move nearer the test type, say to 3  m. If the patient can then just read the 60  m line, the visual acuity, for that particular eye, is 3/60. A visual acuity of less than 1/60 can be recorded as counting fingers (CF), hand movements (HM), perception of light (PL) or no perception of light (NPL). Near vision is tested using reading test types, such as that produced by the UK Faculty of Ophthalmologists (Fig. 11.18). Near visual acuity does not necessarily correlate well with distance acuity.







VISUAL FIELDS


Retinal sensitivity diminishes with increasing distance from the fovea. Visual field mapping defines points in the visual field at which an object of a particular size or illumination is detected. The visual field is not symmetrical. It extends superiorly and medially for approximately 60°, temporally for about 100°, and inferiorly for approximately 75°. The blind spot, situated approximately 15° from fixation in the temporal field, marks the position of the optic disc. The field of vision to a coloured object, reflecting cone function, is more restricted than the field of a white object of the same size. Only the central portions of the two visual fields are binocular, the temporal margins being monocular (Fig. 11.20). Static perimetry involves the detection of a stationary target of varying brightness, while kinetic perimetry involves the detection of a moving target.



To test the visual field sit approximately 1  m from the patient. In infants or poorly cooperating adults, a meaningful response may be impossible to elicit or may be obtained only by using visual threat, in other words, a sudden, unexpected hand movement. For cooperative adults and older children, either finger movements or coloured objects can be used. For testing the left visual field, ask the patient to close or cover the right eye with the right hand, while you cover or close your left eye (Fig. 11.21). Ensure that the patient’s left eye remains fixed on your right eye throughout the examination. The limits of the peripheral field can be determined by bringing the moving fingers of your right hand into the four quadrants of the patient’s field. If individual half fields are full, then the target object, usually your moving fingers, should be presented in both peripheral fields simultaneously. In parietal lobe lesions, particularly of the nondominant hemisphere, a visual target presented in isolation in the contralateral field is perceived but is missed (visual suppression or inattention) when a comparable target is presented simultaneously in the ipsilateral half-field (Fig. 11.22). Peripheral field defects are often detected only if a small target object (e.g. a 10  mm red pin) is used rather than moving fingers.



Stay updated, free articles. Join our Telegram channel

Jul 18, 2017 | Posted by in GENERAL SURGERY | Comments Off on nervous system

Full access? Get Clinical Tree

Get Clinical Tree app for offline access