Extrapolating Animal Safety and Efficacy Data to Humans



Extrapolating Animal Safety and Efficacy Data to Humans






Some of the drugs popular in (country X) and not elsewhere fall into the category of good ideas that lack good data to support their efficacy.

–Lynn Payer, Medicine and Culture.

This chapter describes various types and purposes of extrapolation, and focuses on issues about extrapolation of preclinical safety and efficacy data. General principles of extrapolating data obtained in animals to humans are also presented.


TYPES AND PURPOSES OF EXTRAPOLATIONS

Extrapolation of data can be categorized in several ways. One approach is to focus on three dimensions: the type of organism or species from which the data are obtained, the type of organism or species to which the data are extrapolated, and the type of data involved. Thus, six major types of extrapolation can be described (Table 13.1).

This presentation focuses on the first four types of extrapolation listed in Table 13.1 (i.e., animal safety and efficacy data being extrapolated to other animal species and to humans). The other types of extrapolation are briefly mentioned and discussed more fully in Chapters 89 and 90 of Guide to Clinical Trials (Spilker 1991).

These types of extrapolations can be further divided to discuss extrapolating from:



  • Higher doses to lower doses (e.g., using high-dose findings in animals to comment on the effects that may be observed in humans using much lower doses)


  • Acute treatment to chronic treatment (e.g., using the lethal dose for 50% of animals value to comment on the relative safety of a chronic drug)


  • In vitro to in vivo results (e.g., using in vitro mutagenicity tests to draw conclusions or inferences about carcinogenicity)


  • A healthy animal exposed to a drug to a sick animal from another species (e.g., a healthy rat to a sick patient)


  • An animal of one age exposed to a drug to an animal of another species of a different age (e.g. a young dog to an elderly patient)


  • An animal exposed to a single drug to another species exposed to several drugs (e.g. a rat given a single drug to a human patient taking several different drugs simultaneously)

Each of these aspects is a separate subtype of extrapolations. If each of these subtypes is defined as a dimension, then the more
dimensions one extrapolates across, the less certain the accuracy of the extrapolation is. Numerous biological data are frequently extrapolated across several of these dimensions.








Table 13.1 Types of extrapolations between species































Type of data


From


To


1. Efficacy


Animals


Other animal species


2. Efficacy


Animals


Humans


3. Safety


Animals


Other animal species


4. Safety


Animals


Humans


5. Efficacy


Humans


Other humans


6. Safety


Humans


Other humans


A special case of extrapolation across three dimensions involves determining teratogenic (birth defect) risks. Data are often obtained in healthy animals treated with extremely high doses of a drug. The results of these studies are not only extrapolated to a different species, humans (the first dimension), but are also extrapolated to humans who are receiving relatively small doses of the drug (second dimension) and the human is ill and not healthy (third dimension).

One of the major purposes of extrapolating an interpretation of data is to develop a new hypothesis or model that may be tested. Extrapolations may lead to further experiments or clinical trials that allow compounds or drugs to be tested under different conditions and/or in different types of patients, which allows the accuracy of the extrapolation to be tested. When physicians extrapolate the data from one set or type of patient reported in the literature to one they are treating, they are seeking a therapy to increase the likelihood of improving their patient’s condition.


EXTRAPOLATION OF SAFETY DATA

If the extrapolation of nonclinical safety data could perfectly predict human responses, then most issues concerning extrapolation would not exist. Likewise, if there was absolutely no utility of extrapolating animal data to humans, there would also be little need for as much detailed preclinical investigations as required by regulatory agencies. The actual situation lies in the gray area between these two extremes. The major discussion on extrapolating safety data is organized around the following four questions or issues. Based on the information presented in addressing these questions and also based on data in the literature, a number of principles are then described.



  • How does one evaluate whether nonclinical safety data obtained in cells, tissues, and animals can be extrapolated to humans?


  • How reliable are the nonclinical safety data collected?


  • What do literature data show about extrapolating nonclinical safety data to humans?


  • Are useful drugs being lost because false-positive conclusions are reached when toxic effects observed in laboratory animals are extrapolated to humans?


How Does One Evaluate Whether Nonclinical Safety Data Can Be Extrapolated to Humans?

The most direct approach for determining the extrapolatability of nonclinical results for humans is to measure retrospectively the correlation between results obtained in animals and humans. Even the most accurate data, however, do not enable one to predict whether extrapolation for the next compound tested will yield false-negative, false-positive, or correct conclusions about the effects that will be observed in humans.

One of the biggest reasons why it is difficult to retrospectively investigate the accuracy of extrapolation from nonclinical safety studies to humans is that most drugs that are highly toxic in nonclinical studies are never tested in humans. Thus, the only drugs for which retrospective analysis is possible are those with relatively favorable nonclinical safety profiles. Consequently, retrospective analysis can address the question of how well favorable nonclinical safety results can be extrapolated to humans, but cannot say anything about how well unfavorable nonclinical safety results might predict human safety. This problem makes it extremely difficult to study accurately the predictive value of nonclinical safety studies.


How Reliable Are the Animal Toxicity Data Collected?

This question does not focus on the quality of the data obtained in specific laboratories, although that is sometimes an important consideration. If Good Laboratory Practices regulations are in force, the staff is able and experienced, the facilities are appropriate, and the equipment is up to date, then the quality of the data collected should be acceptable and not be an issue. The issues raised by this question are (a) how consistent are the data obtained, (b) are the numbers of animals used in nonclinical safety studies sufficient to detect uncommon adverse events, and (c) are differences in interpretation of toxicity results among laboratories relatively common and are such differences important? A number of other issues relating to the extrapolation of safety data to humans are discussed in Chapter 88 of Guide to Clinical Trials (Spilker 1991).

If the rate of false positives and false negatives for extrapolating safety data were less than 5%, one might take the position that toxicological data should be accepted as valid, but the larger percentage of false positives and negatives reported in the literature means that the toxicity of all potential drugs must be determined in humans. Nonetheless, only compounds with toxicity profiles that are judged as meeting certain regulatory standards may be ethically tested in humans. Therefore, some potentially valuable drugs are lost because their toxicity in animals is judged greater than what would be acceptable for testing in humans, even though some of those drugs are unlikely to be as toxic in humans as in animals.

Nonclinical safety studies almost always employ relatively few animals, compared to the number of patients from which clinical safety data are obtained. In fact, for most drugs, far more humans are exposed during clinical development than animals exposed during nonclinical development. Consequently, if one assumes that rare adverse events in humans are also rare in animals, then many, if not most uncommon adverse events in humans will not be observed in nonclinical safety studies.

There is an old joke among pathologists that if you get five pathologists together, you get eight separate opinions on
interpreting a histological slide. One of the reasons for this is that no consensus exists among pathologists as to whether pathologists should read tissue slides and interpret specimens blinded or unblinded. The argument for reading slides unblinded states, in part, that knowledge of the clinical diagnosis helps the pathologist better interpret the data, since numerous types of interpretations could usually be made. An extremely defensive editorial in support of unblinded slide reading (Society of Toxicologic Pathology 1986) does not present objective evidence to support its actual position, but ironically presents reasons to support blinding (e.g., “The long-standing practice of open or nonblinded slide reading is based on the fact that morphologic diagnostic pathology is a highly subjective and complex discipline.”) and even fails to consider various methods for blinding slides (e.g., blinding only to treatment group). The argument for blinded reading is based on the notion that the biases that readily enter data analysis and interpretation are minimized. See the paper by Crissman et al. (2004) in the additional readings listing for a recent position paper on this topic.


What Do Literature Data Show about Extrapolating Animal Safety Data?

Ralph Heywood (1990) summarized a correlation of adverse events in humans and animal toxicology data and stated that it was in the range of 5% to 25%. One reason for such poor correlations is that many toxicology studies are conducted using standard study designs without full consideration of how they should be modified to consider human pharmacokinetics, metabolism, and methods of use (e.g., manner of administering the drug or the frequency of administration). Heywood quoted other studies (Heywood 1981; Falahee et al. 1983) showing that the correlation between toxicological results in rats and a non-rodent species was about 30%. Fletcher (1978) predicted, based on 45 drugs studied, that 25% of the toxic effects in animals would occur in humans.

Heywood (1990) states that only four of 22 major adverse events observed in humans since 1960 were predictable from animal studies, and another two adverse events were questionable. It is therefore apparent that most of this group of adverse events could not be predicted using animal studies.

Litchfield (1962) evaluated six compounds studied in humans, rats, and dogs and calculated the likelihood that (a) adverse events would be found in humans if they were found in both rats and dogs and (b) adverse events would not be found in humans if they were only found in one animal species. He found that 68% of the toxic effects observed in both rats and dogs were found in humans and only 21% of toxic effects found in a single animal species were found in humans. He found that for the specific drugs tested, the dog yielded better data than did the rat for predicting human responses (Schein et al. 1961). The best correlations between animal and human data were reported for gastrointestinal complaints, especially vomiting. Schein et al. (1961) reported that Litchfield’s analysis overstated the results by not accounting for the large number of false negatives in animals, which accounted for 68% of the toxicity observed in humans. Selected reasons for false-positive and false-negative observations in toxicology studies are listed in Tables 13.2 and 13.3. Additional discussions on this topic are presented in Animal Toxicity Studies: Their Relevance for Man (Lumley and Walker 1990).








Table 13.2 Selected reasons for false-positive results in toxicologya



































1.


Excessive dosage


2.


Creation of metabolites in animals (but not in humans) that lead to toxicity


3.


Environmental factors favor the lesion, but these factors would not occur in humans.


4.


Species-specific effect unexplained by any of the other factors


5.


Physiological or anatomical differences


6.


Differences in metabolism, distribution, or elimination


7.


Microbial status of the animals differ


8.


Animal housing inappropriate


9.


Diet of animals (e.g., sterile distilled water versus tap water or autoclaved food versus normal animal food)


10.


Technician errors


a Many other reasons discussed in the article by Lumley and Walker (1990) also apply.









Table 13.3 Selected reasons for false-negative responses in toxicology studies









































1.


Species difference (e.g., genetic factors)


2.


Poor absorption


3.


Differences in metabolism or eliminationa


4.


Physiological or anatomical differences


5.


Enzyme induction


6.


Failure to observe subjective symptoms


7.


Failure to observe most skin reactions


8.


Failure to observe hypersensitivity reactions


9.


Absence of the disease and its pathological effects


10.


Failure to measure the effect later found to occur in humans


11.


Differences in microbial status


12.


Underlying pathology of disease in humans exacerbated by drugs in humans, but not observed in animals


a Target organ may not have received sufficient exposure.


Three reasons were given by Johnsson, Ablad, and Hansson (1984) to explain why it is difficult to relate human adverse events to animal data: (a) subjective adverse events are not detectable in animals (e.g., dizziness, headache, and nausea), (b) drug doses (and plasma levels) are often excessive in animal studies, and (c) immunological effects are difficult to detect in animals. A detailed discussion of this topic for a single hepatotoxic drug is given by Clarke et al. (1985).


Oct 2, 2016 | Posted by in GENERAL SURGERY | Comments Off on Extrapolating Animal Safety and Efficacy Data to Humans

Full access? Get Clinical Tree

Get Clinical Tree app for offline access