Chapter 1 Clinical decision making and multiple problem spaces
In the second edition of this book we drew on our initial view of clinical reasoning as a process incorporating the elements of cognition, knowledge and metacognition, expanding this to place a greater emphasis on patient-centred care as the context for clinical reasoning. Practitioners were presented as interactional professionals (Higgs & Hunt 1999) whose effectiveness required interaction with their immediate and larger work environment, with the key players in that context, and with the situational elements pertinent to the patient and case under consideration. Health care was presented via a social ecology model as occurring within the wider sphere of social responsibility of professionals which requires practitioners to be proactive as well as responsive to changes in healthcare contexts (Higgs et al 1999).
In this opening chapter of the third edition we extend our previous examination of the nature of clinical reasoning and its context, drawing on our own research and that of colleagues and co-authors. We expand our interpretation of clinical reasoning from a process view, to explore clinical reasoning as a contextualized phenomenon (see also Chapters 2, 8). We extend consideration of the decision-making context from a focus on the immediate task environment of case management acting in the wider healthcare context to explore the multiple levels of the clinical decision-making space, or rather the multiple decision-making spaces, within which interactive reasoning and decision making occur (see Higgs 2006a, b).
In relation to clinical reasoning expertise, we extend the notion of an expert to encompasscapability, professional artistry and patient-centredness; expertise is a journey rather than a point of arrival (see also Chapters 11 and 16). In examining and making explicit these aspects of clinical reasoning our goal is to make clinical reasoning more accessible for novices to learn, for experienced practitioners to portray, for educators to teach, for clinicians to practise and for researchers to explore.
UNDERSTANDING CLINICAL REASONING
In the 10 years since we produced the first edition of this book, we have retained our view that clinical reasoning is both simple and complex. Simply, clinical reasoning is the sum of the thinking and decision-making processes associated with clinical practice; it is a critical skill in the health professions, central to the practice of professional autonomy, and it enables practitioners to take ‘wise’ action, meaning taking the best judged action in a specific context (Cervero 1988, Harris 1993). Despite being straightforward and ‘simple’ this view is very broad; clinical reasoning is seen as permeating throughout clinical practice and as being the core of practice. The importance of understanding the complex nature of clinical reasoning is emphasized in the goal of developing tolerance of ambiguity and a reflexive understanding of practice artistry during health sciences education, as suggested by Bleakley et al (2003).
CLINICAL REASONING AND METASKILLS
Our previous model of clinical reasoning (Higgs & Jones 2000) was presented as an upward and outward spiral, a cyclical and a developing process. Each loop of the spiral incorporated data input, data interpretation (or reinterpretation) and problem formulation (or reformulation) to achieve a progressively broader and deeper understanding of the clinical problem. Based on this deepeningunderstanding, decisions are made concerning intervention, and actions are taken. The process was described as including:
To these dimensions we now add four meta-skills:
THE ADEQUACY OF DIFFERENT INTERPRETATIONS
THE NATURE OF CLINICAL REASONING AS A PHENOMENON
DIFFERENT INTERPRETATIONS OF CLINICAL REASONING
In various chapters of this book a number of interpretations of clinical reasoning are discussed from the perspective of different disciplines, the history of clinical reasoning research, and models of practice within which clinical reasoning occurs. In Table 1.1 we present an overview of key models,strategies and interpretations of clinical reasoning. These have been divided into two groups: cognitive and interactive models. This division reflects three trends: changes in the focus of research and theoretical understandings of clinical reasoning (see Chapters 18, 19); changes in society and expectations of health care (see Chapter 2); and a major shift in emphasis (as outlined above) from the second to the third edition of this book.
